Skip to main content

Currently Skimming:


Pages 105-131

The Chapter Skim interface presents what we've algorithmically identified as the most significant single chunk of text within every page in the chapter.
Select key terms on the right to highlight them within pages of the chapter.


From page 105...
... 105 Reliability information encompasses a broad range of information that describes underlying trip variability and other contextual data travelers use to manage delay and on-time performance. Included in this category are (a)
From page 106...
... 106 evaluated in the experiment were selected on the basis of the lexicon findings and accepted by the SHRP 2 L14 technical expert task group. The specific reliability-related terms, phrases, and delivery media assessed in this experiment are listed as follows: 1.
From page 107...
... 107 questions, followed by the group-led experiment and varying post-experiment surveys. Appendix G lists the specific data types requested and generated from the pre-experiment survey questions.
From page 108...
... 108 duration based on the selected departure. For example, a 30-min trip required the participant to view progress over 6 s, while a 60-min trip required the participant to view progress over 12 s.
From page 109...
... 109 participant experienced four of the seven reliability-related data, a specific term each for a week. During one week, participants received no reliability-related data.
From page 110...
... 110 • End-of-week helpfulness of information in reducing commute stress (1 = not at all, 5 = absolutely) ; • End-of-week usefulness of information overall (1 = not useful, 5 = very useful)
From page 111...
... 111 to 49 years. Close to half of all participants had a college degree or higher, whereas only 25% of the U.S.
From page 112...
... 112 medical destinations was less frequently cited as very important. Between 64% and 68% of individuals making school, child-care, and medical trips indicated that arriving on time was very important for their trips.
From page 113...
... 113 work trips by car, 42% had an average work-trip time that was similar to the times presented in the experiment (40 min or greater) , while another 45% traveled slightly lower work-trip times, ranging from 20 min to 40 min.
From page 114...
... 114 guaranteed travel time mitigates adverse trip arrival outcomes, while the knowledge of a late arrival does not change an adverse trip outcome. experiment 1 Findings: effectiveness and Valuation of reliability Terms for Unfamiliar Trips Participants in this experiment made two valuations and a departure time decision pre-trip, as well as three valuations at the end of each simulated commute.
From page 115...
... 115 Ratings for information usefulness when asked post-trip were either lower than the baseline or otherwise not significantly different, even when trip outcomes were better with the reliability information. One reason for this phenomenon may be that when participants were late, they faulted more strongly the reliability information if available; however, in the absence of reliability information, they may have faulted inherent traffic rather than the baseline radio information.
From page 116...
... 116 Text-Based Average and 95th Percentile Reliability Information Versus Baseline (Type C Versus Type A) Among all experiment participants, 185 individuals made a week (5 days)
From page 117...
... 117 When asked at the end of the week, individuals were willing to pay on average $0.22 more for type C information compared with the baseline; however, for the same question at the end of each trip, participants were willing to pay about $0.31 more per trip. Finally, when asked how difficult it was to understand the traveler information, participants rated type C slightly more challenging to understand (2.2 versus 2.0)
From page 118...
... 118 Conflicting outcomes were observed between post-trip and end-of-week valuations for information usefulness and effectiveness in reducing stress. Participants gave the reliability information type D a higher usefulness rating pre-trip and at the end of the week, but a lower rating post-trip compared with the no reliability information week ratings.
From page 119...
... 119 bar chart showing estimated travel time and extra time for unexpected delays, using departure time as the x-axis and arrival time as the y-axis. The graphic is presented in Appendix H, Figure H.6.
From page 120...
... 120 information; however, this difference was not statistically significant. That is, participants did not find the graphical reliability information any more difficult to understand than the radio message without reliability information.
From page 121...
... 121 format (WAV) files played while participants wore headphones.
From page 122...
... 122 (type H) , while the information in types C and D was presented in graphical formats (type F and type G, respectively)
From page 123...
... 123 text-based 95th percentile reliability information (type B) and another week (5 days)
From page 124...
... 124 Participants, when using the text-based information, chose to depart on average slightly earlier (on average 2 min earlier) , and consequently arrived on average 3.1 min earlier compared with when they had access to the graphical form of the same information.
From page 125...
... 125 experiment 2 Findings: reliability data expedites Learning Curve for Trip Familiarity The objective of experiment 2 was to determine whether having reliability information helped speed the transition from an unfamiliar traveler to an experienced commuter. The underlying conjecture was that providing reliability information allows an unfamiliar traveler to manage the trade-off between on-time performance and travel budget (time allocated to travel)
From page 126...
... 126 and travel budget (time allocated to travel) more effectively.
From page 127...
... 127 The second factor focused on the individual, while the first factor focused on the general. Individuals were expected to rate their ease of understanding information more positively, even when rating the complexity of the information as high.
From page 128...
... 128 95th percentile reliability terms. Third was the graphical average and 95th percentile reliability terms.
From page 129...
... 129 Figure 10.8. Rank ordering of reliability terms based on information usefulness.
From page 130...
... 130 Researchers developed three hypotheses, and findings related to each of these hypotheses are described below. Hypothesis 1 stated that provision of accurate reliability information (in an easy-to-understand format)
From page 131...
... 131 and stress reduction. To some degree, this was because of the complexity of the presentation and the brevity with which participants were required to learn and interpret information content.

Key Terms



This material may be derived from roughly machine-read images, and so is provided only to facilitate research.
More information on Chapter Skim is available.